Psychophysics
Introduction to Psychophysics


Historical Setting

About 150 years ago (October 22, 1850) a German philosopher, Gustav Fechner, thought he saw a solution to problem posed by philosophers for years. For centuries, many philosophers had adopted a dualistic way of looking at the world. There was the physical world and there was the world of the mind--the physical and the mental (or psychic, or spiritual). The body belonged to the physical world and consciousness belonged to the mental world. Illusions of various sorts had shown philosophers that the laws of the physical and the laws of the mental worlds were different. Nevertheless in some way the worlds are intimately related. Our everyday experience argues that this immaterial mind in some way very efficaciously controls the material body. How are these two "realities" related? Descartes, Spinoza, Liebnitz, and Berkeley are just a few of the persons who had given serious thought to this matter.

Fechner saw how it was possible to measure sensations--an integral part of the mental world--in terms of stimulation--the part and parcel of the physical world. He termed the study of the relationship between the psychological and the physical "realities" psychophysics. The methods he developed for measuring the relationship he called the "Psychophysical Methods".

In a way Fechner wanted to do something similar to what Einstein did about 50 years later:

Current Philosophical Basis of Psychophysics

Psychologists still use the procedures developed by Fechner and they are still called psychophysical methods. But what's being measured is not the relationship between mind and matter.

Sensation instead is considered as a hypothetical construct. We measure stimuli and certain types of behaviors that occur after the stimulus has been presented to the individual. To explain the consistency of various behaviors scientists have postulated the concept of sensation--an event that happens inside the animal. Sensation is a hypothetical construct, it is not an element of conscious experience which in the past was called sensation. The term "sensation" is now a technical term and does not have the meaning given to the word when it is used in everyday, nontechnical conversations.

This change in meaning is very important to remember. In testing we talk about the validity of a test--to what extent does the test actually measure what we see it measures. There are several ways that we can go about actually measuring validity. One type of validity is that of face validity. All face validity does is ask, "Does this test convince the subject that it measures what it says it measures?" Some tests have unscored items that increase face validity but if those items were scores would make the test less valid. Many concepts in the basic areas of psychology are tricky for we use as a technical concept an everyday word. What we unconsciously do is try to use the word both ways at the same time. Sometimes it is possible to do this and sometimes it is not possible. Sometimes we can read the word "sensation" in the text and think of our conscious content--at that moment, for that context, the word "sensation" has face validity. Other times, the term "sensations" will be used and it cannot be referenced to your conscious content. In this second usage the term does not have face validity. Nevertheless, in the second case "sensation: may be a good hypothetical construct for it may be doing a find job of integrating and predicting--and for science's criteria, that makes the term useful.

Fechner's Revelation

It had been recognized for many many centuries that sensation was not the same as stimulation. Measuring the stimulus does NOT also measure our response to the stimulus (or "does NOT also measure our internal response to the stimulus which we'll call the 'sensation' produced by the stimulus").

Philosophers had stated for many years that you couldn't measure sensation. Two of the reasons were:

Fechner woke up one morning (October 22, 1850) with a "Eureka! I know how to measure sensation. We'll measure it the same way we measure everything else in physics. We will measure it by the null matching procedure!"

Null matching is the fundamental observational procedure in science.
  • A null-match is achieved when one stimulus is adjusted (made weaker or stronger) until it looks exactly the same as another stimulus.
  • The best agreement (both inter-observer and intra-observer) can be achieved if a given observation can be set up as a null-match or "no difference" judgment.
  • Science begins with simple instruments and the null match. Instruments are designed to give the same results as trained observers' null matching. The useful aspect of the instruments is that an untrained observer can now obtain a value that agrees with a trained scientist.
  • Fechner's "Method of Adjustment" procedure simply standardizes the presentation of the stimuli being judged to reduce observational error or at least to make them balance out.

Example: Its 1830. You are a physicist studying light. You want to know how the amount of light from a candle is changed by using different types of wax because its getting harder and harder to buy the wax you have been using.

  • Compare the brightness (a sensation) of a candle made with the replacement wax (the unknown) at 1 foot distance with the brightness of a candle made with the current wax (known).
  • Arrange the candles so the known candle illuminates half of a square of paper and the unknown illuminates the other half.
  • Move the known candle closer or further away until the two half-squares are equally bright.
  • The relative distances of the two light sources when the illuminated squares are equally bright can be used to specify how many times the light energy of the unknown stimulus is to the known stimulus.

Example: Titration in Chemistry. How acidic (What is its pH?) is this liquid. Add indicator substance to a liquid. Have an example of the endpoint available. Add acid and base in known amounts until the sample you are titrating has the same color as the endpoint. Since the color (or appearance) of the two test tubes match, the pH of the unknown liquid now is the same as that of the end point. By working backward (how much acid or base did you add) you can specify the pH before the additions.

Physicists didn't try to measure amounts by seeing if one thing was twice something else or 4 times something else. Make it the same. If you wanted 2 times, you'd put two standard candles down and do null matching.

Over the years instruments were developed which gave the same readings as a trained obsever's null match. The useful aspect of the instruments is that an untrained observer can now obtain the value that agrees with the trained scientist.

  • Nowadays highschool chemistry labs have pH meters. You dip the probe in the liquid and directly read the pH-value. Naturally the pH meters were built to give the same readings that highly skilled chemists achieved with the null matching procedure known as "Titration".

Fechner agreed that there was no way you could ever say that one sensation was four times larger than some other sensation. That there was no way to say that one light is twice as bright an some other light. What Fechner did was to discover that what we can do is to say whether or not a sensation is present and whether or not two sensations are the same or not. These two judgments formed the basis of the field we call classical psychophysics. What Fechner did was to say that you can specify a sensation in terms of the stimulus values needed to produce certain types of sensations--actually certain types of judgments. From these landmark stimuli, we can build the whole structure which will allow us to say that one light is twice as bright as another.

Problem Areas of Interest to Classical Psychophysics

Threshold determinations--liminal values of stimuli: What amounts of stimuli produce certain types of behavior.

Additional Psychophysical Interests

An interesting addendum


©2002 by Burrton Woodruff. All rights reserved. Modified Friday, June 7, 2002